Thursday, November 27, 2014

Lies My Turkey Told Me: Tryptophan

Learning By Doing

The molecular structure of L-TryptophanThis week's activity is pretty easy. Grab a turkey leg (or spirulina or cashews if you're vegan), and take a big bite. Now wait while your body processes the food. Make a mental note of the time of day, any other food you may have eaten, and how alert you feel.

From Gut to Grey Matter

In this special holiday edition, I wanted to cover a topic related to Thanksgiving. The choice was obvious. I need to blog about Tryptophan! I'm sure you've probably heard the following explanation for why we get sleepy after a massive Thanksgiving feast. Turkey is full of essential proteins, fatty acids, and nutrients. In particular, turkey is said to contain an unusually large amount of Tryptophan, which is a necessary amino acid. Amino acids, as you may recall, combine with other compounds to create proteins that your body can use.

It turns out that Tryptophan is a biological or chemical precursor for other neurotransmitters. The body isn't able to produce its own Tryptophan, so it must be supplied by the food that we eat. Enter the drumstick. Tryptophan, then, is chemically altered by the body to form Serotonin, which is a neurotransmitter that serves many different functions. One such function is to regulate mood (it is associated with feelings of contentment and happiness) and sleep as well. Serotonin, in turn, can be converted into Melatonin, which is a hormone the brain sends the body to tell it that it's time to go to bed [1].

In a nutshell, here's the chemical chain of events


Tryptophan → Serotonin → Melatonin

Tryprophan is a precursor for Serotonin, which gets converted to Melatonin, which then tells the body to go to sleep. That seems logical. Turkey makes us sleepy because if fuels this bio-chemical waterfall.


Armchair Neuroscience: Getting off the couch

Ah...but there's a huge problem with this explanation. Actually, there are a couple of problems. 

First, feeling sleepy after a large Thanksgiving dinner is hopelessly confounded with the time in which people typically eat. The body has a natural rhythm (called a "circadian rhythm") in which people feel wide awake and alert, and other times of the day when we feel sleepy and tired. For most people, there's a natural dip in the afternoon (siesta, anyone?).

Second, eating a large meal containing a glut of protein is a taxing process that your body has to then deal with. Most of the blood in your extremities goes to your stomach to aid in the digestive process and to carry off the newly absorbed nutrients. What better way to pass the time than dozing off for an hour or so?

Finally, the chemical process outlined above, while true, tends to take some time. I would be shocked if this chemical conversion process happens between the time you push away from the table and pass out on the couch. 

The S.T.E.M. Connection

Wouldn't it be cool if we were teaching a chemistry class, and we could synthesize melatonin in the lab? A more likely connection might be to ask a science class to use the scientific method to confirm or disconfirm a hypothesis that everyone seems to take for granted (e.g., "turkey causes people to become sleepy because it contains tryptophan"). 

That could also lead to an interesting discussion about applying critical thinking to claims that sound "scientific." We could then discuss the following questions: 
  • How do you know turkey causes sleepiness? 
  • What other foods cause people to become tired? 
  • What other foods that contain tryptophan don't generally induce sleep? 
  • How do you counteract alternative explanations, such as time-of-day effects (e.g., circadian rhythms) and other confounding factors (e.g., the size of the meal or the amount of protein)? 
I admit...I thought that I was going to end up blogging about how turkey makes people sleepy. Then again, I believe all sorts of lies that my teachers, family members, tv shows, movies, and friends have taught me. I just need to remember to keep asking myself, "How do I know that it's true? What's the evidence? Is it any good?"


Share and Enjoy!

Dr. Bob


Going Beyond the Information Given

[1] It's useful to distinguish between hormones and neurotransmittersA neurotransmitter is a chemical that cells in the brain use to communicate with other cells or regions of the brain; whereas, a hormone is how the brain communicates with the rest of the body. 

Thursday, November 20, 2014

Priming the Pump: Semantic & Perceptual Priming

Musicum Revelio!

 I have this amazing app installed on my iPad, called Shazam. It can identify pretty much any song that's ever been recorded. I have no idea how it works. As far as I'm concerned, it operates on magic. I shared my theory with a colleague, and he said it probably uses an algorithm called nearest neighbor. Ok, so maybe magic is the wrong explanation. Instead, the app uses a collection of features (e.g., beats per minute, key, and vocal range) to classify the song. That got me thinking...our brain does a similar bit of magic everyday, all the time! Any time you see someone you know or recognize their voice, you just did a quick bit of classification based on very little information. Not only that, it happens almost instantaneously. The brain is super fast at classification. 

If our brain is so freakin' awesome, why does it blow it every once in a while? Have you ever had the experience of recognizing someone's face, but you have no idea what the person's name is or any biographical information about them? You know that you know them, but you don't know how you know them. It usually happens when you see that person out of context. For example, you may see a coworker, but they are at the grocery store instead of the office. It takes longer to recognize them out of context. Why is that the case? 


"Lake Superior! That's the answer to the first question!" —Lane Maxwell

Our mind uses a trick to speed up processing of incoming information. It helps boost efficiency by working within the same semantic space. In other words, classification works better when you know where to look. For example, in the gameshow Wheel of Fortune, they always give the contestants the category before they start solving the puzzle. The categories are often vague (e.g., "before and after" or "around the house"), but at least you don't have to comb through your entire body of knowledge to identify the words. In other words, knowing the category primes you to think about certain things. Eliminating huge swaths of information can help increase processing speeds. Thus, Priming is the phenomena where early information helps speed up processing of later information [1].

How does priming work? If you've been reading this blog, then you can probably anticipate my answer. The Associative Network and Spreading Activation can help explain how priming works. Once a node in the network is active, activation will spread to its nearest neighbors first, and then radiate out to other, related concepts. I would predict that you would be able to identify the song ABC quickly when, right before the song came on, we were talking about the Jackson family. Indeed, this makes intuitive sense. Getting in the mindset helps you become more accurate and faster at processing new information. 

Another example appeared in a previous post where I attempted to use priming to help the reader solve a puzzle. The goal is to find the common connection between three unrelated words: blue, cake, and cottage. Later, on that page, I had a picture of a piece of swiss cheese. My hope was that the image would prime the reader to figure out that the connection between blue, cake, and cottage is cheese. Priming can be our friend. 

There is, however, a downside. Priming doesn't work at a conscious level. In other words, priming happens outside of our awareness. Why would this be a bad thing? One reason why it can be a disservice is that we don't always give proper credit, or attribution, to the source of our ideas. We may think we are being creative and coming up with our own ideas. But as this amazing video demonstrates, that isn't always the case [2].


A STEM Example

How can we use the idea of Priming to help enhance education? It's tough to exploit, mainly because priming operates outside of our conscious awareness. However, a creative educator might engineer a lesson so that she can prime students to answer a logical chain of questions. 

Our stats teacher in college did something like this. We didn't know it at the time, but the lesson was about calculating the standard deviation of a sample. Instead of putting the formula on the board and asking us to memorize it, he started by talking about something (seemingly) unrelated: linear transformations. How would you shift the mean of a set of data, represented as a vertical line, up or down the x-axis? Once we got good at that, he asked us another question: How far away, on average, is each point from the mean? That got us thinking about the spread of the data, and he drew upon something we already knew: how to compute the mean. Finally, we noticed that the difference between the data points were both positive and negative. So we had to figure out a way to standardize that. 

Asking students leading questions, and allowing them to explore the problem space in a structured way, is a good way to exploit the power of priming in education. I am curious to hear in the comments section other ways that we can harness the positive power of priming in education. 

Share and Enjoy! 

Dr. Bob


For More Information

[1] Priming is a very cool concept, but how do we know it's real? What is the scientific evidence that convinced the field that priming is a property of the mind? Early evidence came from a study that asked people to judge whether a string of letters were words (butter) or not (plame). When words were semantically related (e.g., "butter" and "bread"), participants were faster to respond than when they were not related (e.g., "butter" and "nurse"). Of course, we're talking about a difference of 85 milliseconds, but still! That study gave us early evidence that priming was real. 

     Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence upon retrieval operationsJournal of Experimental Psychology, 90, 227–234.


[2] Other real-life examples abound in Walter Isaacson's fascinating book The Innovators. His historical analysis of the computer demonstrates time and again that inventors downplay the influence they received by talking with other people or by looking at their prototypes. When a lot of money is at stake, people conveniently ignore the impact of priming on their inventions.



Thursday, November 13, 2014

Covering the Spread: Spreading Activation


"Oh, rats!" —Indiana Jones

Let's play a game. What do the following three things have in common?   


blue       cake       cottage

One is a color, the second is a type of dessert, and the last is a little house in the woods. It doesn't seem like they have much in common. But there is one concept that binds them together. Keep thinking about it. Or don't! Sometimes the best way to see the connection between (seemingly) related things is to leave it alone and let your mind engage in some background processing.

Switching gears a moment...let's talk about what it means to be "reminded" of something. For example, I had lunch with one of my coworkers, and she told me about this man who randomly stopped by her house when she wasn't home, and he left some candy on her front porch. Her story reminded me of a movie that my wife and I recently watched about a guy who wants to be a freelance journalist. So wait a minute...What does a guy leaving candy at my friend's house have to do with a movie about journalism? Well, the movie is entitled Nightcrawler, and the connection I saw was "anti-social behavior" (or maybe even "mental illness!"). This type of thing happens all the time in conversation. Something one person says reminds another person about a completely different topic. How does that happen?


Back to the Network

One potential explanation is to return to an idea that was introduced in a previous postI made the claim that an Associative Network is a very powerful way to represent someone's knowledge. It is powerful because it can explain other cognitive phenomena, such as "reminding." When we say something "reminds you" of something else, what are we talking about? And how can we use that information to map someone's knowledge?

One of the properties of a network is called the "connection strength" (or proximity) between two concepts (or nodes). For example, apples are strongly associated with bananas because they are both types of fruit. But apples are only very remotely associated with the Kentucky Derby. (It's a long walk, but you can imagine the following chain of associations: Horses eat apples, which give them energy to run, and people like to watch horses race at the Kentucky Derby.) 

That means something can remind us of another thing either by the strength of the connection between them or the number of hops you need to connect any two concepts. Back to the original question: According to this theory, how does "reminding" work? The theory states that each node is connected to one (or many) other nodes. When that node becomes active, due to some stimulus in the environment, activation spreads throughout the network of ideas. Back to our fruit example, if I see an apple, then activation spreads out to other fruit, including bananas, and continues to radiate outward to other concepts. Spreading Activation, then, is the idea that one node becomes active, which activates  another node, which then activates a third node, and so on until the activation dies out.


A STEM Example

I like the idea of an associative network of ideas because, as an educator, you can start to bootstrap your lessons based on what your students already know. A perfect example is Netwon's Law Universal of Gravitation, which is summarized by the following equation:


F=(G*m_1*m_2)/r^2


F represents the force between two objects (e.g., the sun and the Earth), G is a constant, m is the mass of the first and second object, and r is the distance between them. It is extremely helpful to know this particular equation when students later learn Coulomb's law, which describes the force experienced by two charged particles:


Notice anything? There are subtle differences between the two laws, but the overall structure of the equations is remarkably similar. In fact, when teaching Coulomb's law, it is helpful to ask the students if they are reminded of anything from their previous lessons.


Back to the Beginning

I opened this post with a "game." It's origin isn't a game, but a test of creativity called the "Remote Associates Test" (or "R.A.T." for short). The idea is that creative individuals have many connections between nodes and when activation spreads, it hits remote parts of the network. This makes intuitive sense because creative people are most likely to be described as "divergent thinkers." Now we have a way to visualize what divergent means. Maybe we can even train ourselves to be more creative by not stopping at the first thing that you are reminded of. Instead, force yourself to keep activating other parts of your associative network.

Share and Enjoy!

Dr. Bob



For More Information


You can test your creativity by taking the RAT here. Also, remember the overused phrase, "think outside the box"? Ever wonder where that came from? According to internet lore, its origin is found in the "nine dot problem" where you have to connect all of the dots with only 4 lines. Try it! 




Thursday, November 6, 2014

Work the Network: Associative Networks

Contradictions in Memory

Our intuition about how memory works says that you can only remember a couple of things at a time, right? For example, if I start rattling off a grocery list, you might want to start jotting things down after I list the fourth fruit or vegetable. 

So here’s the conundrum: Why does memory get better when we start adding additional information? That sounds like a contradiction, right? Absolutely! But there’s a good reason why it works, and it has everything to do with the way memory is structured. 

Our memory system is a fascinating knot of complementary (and often contradictory!) mechanisms. We need these different systems because our environment is sufficiently complex. We are confronted with many different tasks that include different sources of information. If you have a quick task that will only take a few seconds, then you need a fast memory system that inhales information and spits it out quickly. However, most of the interesting things that we do require us to remember something over a long period of time. You might call that “learning.”

How, then, can we enhance our learning? How can we make sure the information that we see or hear gets cemented in long-term memory? One memory hack is to start adding all sorts of details that will help enhance the memory that you want to form. Here’s an example from my own life. 

What's in a name?

A few weeks ago, I met one of my new coworkers. I had no problem remembering her first name, but her last name escaped me. It’s embarrassing when you can’t remember someone’s name, even when you try. I needed help, and here’s what I came up with. 

I am a hockey fan, and in college I started following the Detroit Red Wings. They have a history of recruiting promising players from other countries. While these players might not shine during their first year, the Red Wings sign them for extended contracts and commit to developing their talent. A perfect example is when the Red Wings signed Pavel Datsyuk in 2001. 

So what does a forward for the Red Wings have to do with remembering my coworker’s name? Well, the first five letters of her last name are “Pavel” (plus some additional letters at the end). In effect, what I did was add a bunch of seemingly irrelevant information to help me remember her last name. I made an effort to embed her name in a larger network of information. Moreover, when I try to recall her name, I have several hooks to get me to the right name. I can think about the field of Cognitive Science, Hockey, or work, and all routes should lead me to the desired destination. 

Why does that work? Or said another way, what does the structure of long-term memory look like? I have no idea, mainly because it is so fluid and multi-faceted. However, one way Cognitive Scientists have attempted to visualize the complexity our memory is to use a node-link structure called an Associative Network. A small portion of my network probably looks like this:




Each node is a concept and a link between them is an “association.” In other words, each concept reminds me of the other nodes connected to it. For example, when I think of John, I am reminded of Chas (and vice versa). The degree to which concepts are connected also matters. The distance between Hockey and Cognitive Science is remote. So they shouldn’t remind me of each other.

A STEM Example

This has obvious implications for education. For example, suppose you were teaching a biology class to a group of young children. They know the definition of a “mammal,” and they can give many examples (e.g., cats and dogs) and counter examples (e.g., birds and fish). When they first learned about mammals, they learned that mammals have a couple of defining characteristics: They breath air; they have fur or hair; they have 3 inner-ear bones, they give birth to live offspring; and nurse their young. Most kids at this age, however, incorrectly classify a whale as a type of fish. That means they think whales don’t have hair, don’t give birth to live young (or nurse them for that matter!). 

In essence, what you have done as a teacher is completely broken one of the links in their Associative Network and moved it over. Thus, learning might look like this:


==becomes==>


An Associative Network representation helps demonstrate the importance of prior knowledge on learning. It also helps explain other cognitive phenomena like priming, cued recall, and spreading activation (all of which will be the topics of future posts). 

Share and Enjoy!

Dr. Bob


For more information

Here is my favorite empirically derived network. It depicts an expert child's representation of her dinosaur knowledge. You can see that some dinosaurs hang together tightly, while others are more remotely associated. Also, the number of links between them also shows the strength of the connection between the two animals. 


Used with permission from the author.


Source: Chi, M. T. H., & Koeske, R. D. (1983). Network representation of a child’s dinosaur knowledge. Developmental Psychology, 19(1), 29–39.